Cognitive planning in the context of neuropsychological assessment and research in cognitive neuroscience
PDF (Español (España))

Keywords

planning
executive functions
neuropsychological assessment
prefrontal cortex planificación
funciones ejecutivas
evaluación neuropsicológica
corteza prefrontal

How to Cite

Domic-Siede , M., Irani , M., Ramos-Henderson, M., Calderón, C., Ossandón, T., & Perrone-Bertolotti , M. (2023). Cognitive planning in the context of neuropsychological assessment and research in cognitive neuroscience: a systematic review. Terapia Psicológica, 40(3), 367-395. Retrieved from https://teps.cl/index.php/teps/article/view/548

Abstract

Planning is defined as the ability to develop a sequenced plan of behavioral steps to achieve a goal and is part of a set of high-order cognitive functions called executive functions. This function is affected in various daily life situations and in a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders. The design of cognitive tests for their use in the clinical context and the design of experimental paradigms for assessment in research coupled with neuroimaging techniques to elucidate their neural bases, continue to be a challenge for clinical neuropsychology and neurosciences. In this PRISMA systematic review article, we review theory and research regarding clinical assessment and research into the neurobiological bases of planning and contributions to understanding the mechanisms of its implementation. Common methodological measures are reported and the theoretical approaches that contribute to their understanding are summarized. Further clinical, instrumental, and experimental studies are needed to better understand planning in the context of an integrative theory of executive functions and the role of the prefrontal cortex.

PDF (Español (España))

References

Alchihabi, A., Ekmekci, O., Kivilcim, B. B., Newman, S. D., & Yarman Vural, F. T. (2021). Analyzing Complex Problem Solving by Dynamic Brain Networks. Frontiers in neuroinformatics, 15, 670052. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2021.670052

Allain, P., Nicoleau, S., Pinon, K., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., Barré, J., Berrut, G., Dubas, F., & Le Gall, D. (2005). Executive functioning in normal aging: a study of action planning using the Zoo Map Test. Brain and cognition, 57(1), 4–7. https://doi.org/bx4fqp

Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications (5a ed). Worth.

Baddeley A. D. (2021). Developing the Concept of Working Memory: The Role of Neuropsychology. Archives of clinical neuropsychology (Online), 36(6), 861–873. https://doi.org/gp2rg9

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485–493. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485

Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the Central Executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(1), 5-28. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755608

Bailey C. E. (2007). Cognitive accuracy and intelligent executive function in the brain and in business. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118 (1), 122–141. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1412.011

Baker, S. C., Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Frith, C. D., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: A PET study of the tower of London task. Neuropsychologia, 34(6),515-526. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00133-6

Balaguer, J., Spiers, H., Hassabis, D., & Summerfield, C. (2016). Neural Mechanisms of Hierarchical Planning in a Virtual Subway Network. Neuron, 20(4), 893-903. https://doi.org/gffg4j

Barkley, R. A. (2004). Adolescents with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: An Overview of Empirically Based Treatments. Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 10(1), 39-56. https://doi.org/10.1097/00131746-200401000-00005

Benson, D. F. (1993). Prefrontal abilities. Behavioural Neurology, 6(2), 75-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/BEN-1993-6202

Berman, K. F., Ostrem, J. L., Randolph, C., Gold, J., Goldberg, T. E., Coppola, R., Carson, R. E., Herscovitch, P., & Weinberger, D. R. (1995). Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia, 33(8), 1027–1046. https://doi.org/d37rrc

Bora, E., Harrison, B. J., Yücel, M., & Pantelis, C. (2013). Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 43(10), 2017 – 2026. https://doi.org/f49qq2

Boschin, E. A., & Buckley, M. J. (2015). Differential contributions of dorsolateral and frontopolar cortices to working memory processes in the primate. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00144

Boyer, B. E., Geurts, H. M., & Van der Oord, S. (2018). Planning Skills of Adolescents With ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 22(1), 46-57. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054714538658

Brown, T. E., & Landgraf, J. M. (2010). Improvements in executive function correlate with enhanced performance and functioning and health-related quality of life: evidence from 2 large, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trials in ADHD. Postgraduate medicine, 122(5), 42–51. https://doi.org/10.3810/pgm.2010.09.2200

Brown, J. W. (2017). Models of Anterior Cingulate Cortex Function in Cognitive Control. En T. Egner (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control (pp. 259–273). Wiley https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118920497.ch15

Brown, T. I., Carr, V. A., LaRocque, K. F., Favila, S. E., Gordon, A. M., Bowles, B., Bailenson, J. N., & Wagner, A. D. (2016). Prospective representation of navigational goals in the human hippocampus. Science, 352(6291), 1323–1326. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf0784

Burgess, P. W., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. (2007). The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 290-298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.004

Burgess, P. W., Quayle, A., & Frith, C. D. (2001). Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography. Neuropsychologia, 39(6), 545–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00149-4

Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., De Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38(6),848-863. https://doi.org/dz8pbn

Buzsáki, G., & Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science, 304(5679), 1926-1929. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1099745

Campbell, Z., Zakzanis, K. K., Jovanovski, D., Joordens, S., Mraz, R., & Graham, S. J. (2009). Utilizing virtual reality to improve the ecological validity of clinical neuropsychology: an fMRI case study elucidating the neural basis of planning by comparing the Tower of London with a three-dimensional navigation task. Applied Neuropsychology, 16(4), 295–306. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084280903297891

Carlin, D., Bonerba, J., Phipps, M., Alexander, G., Shapiro, M., & Grafman, J. (2000). Planning impairments in frontal lobe dementia and frontal lobe lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 655-665. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00102-5

Cavanagh, J. F., & Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 414-421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

Christoff, K., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2000). The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: Evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex. Psychobiology, 28(2), 168–186. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03331976

Cohen, J. D. (2017). The basics of cognitive control: Core constructs and current considerations. En T. Egner (Ed.), The Wiley Handbook of Cognitive Control (pp. 1-28). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118920497.ch1

Collins, A., & Koechlin, E. (2012). Reasoning, learning, and creativity: frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS biology, 10(3), e1001293. https://doi.org/gfkbbk

Colvin, M. K., Dunbar, K., & Grafman, J. (2001). The effects of frontal lobe lesions on goal achievement in the Water Jug task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(8), 1129-1147. https://doi.org/dtvd9j

Crowe, D. A., Averbeck, B. B., Chafee, M. V., Anderson, J. H., & Georgopoulos, A. P. (2000). Mental maze solving. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 813-827. https://doi.org/dw6c6f

Dagher, A., Owen, A. M., Boecker, H., & Brooks, D. J. (1999). Mapping the network for planning: A correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task. Brain, 122(10), 1973-1987. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.10.1973

Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078. https://doi.org/bfs49w

Davis, J. C., Marra, C. A., Najafzadeh, M., & Liu-Ambrose, T. (2010). The independent contribution of executive functions to health related quality of life in older women. BMC geriatrics, 10; 16. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2318-10-16

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135-168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental cognitive neuroscience, 18, 34–48. https://doi.org/gftd6n

Donoso, M., Collins, A. G. E., & Koechlin, E. (2014). Foundations of human reasoning in the prefrontal cortex. Science, 344(6191), 1481-1486. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1252254

Domic-Siede M., Irani M., Valdés J., Perrone-Bertolotti M., Ossandón T. (2021). Theta activity from frontopolar cortex, mid-cingulate cortex and anterior cingulate cortex shows different roles in cognitive planning performance. NeuroImage, 226;117557. https://doi.org/gk32f4

Dreher, J. C., Koechlin, E., Tierney, M., & Grafman, J. (2008). Damage to the fronto-polar cortex is associated with impaired multitasking. PLoS ONE, 3(9), e3227. https://doi.org/brjm7t

Dreher, M., & Oerter, R. (1987). Action planning competencies during adolescence and early adulthood. En S. L. Friedman, E. K. Scholnick, & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), Blueprints for thinking: The role of planning in cognitive development (pp. 321–355). Cambridge University.

Eakin, L., Minde, K., Hechtman, L., Ochs, E., Krane, E., Bouffard, R., Greenfield, B., & Looper, K. (2004). The marital and family functioning of adults with ADHD and their spouses. Journal of attention disorders, 8(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1177/108705470400800101

Evans, J. S. B. T., Over, D. E., & Manktelow, K. I. (1993). Reasoning, decision making and rationality. Cognition, 49(1-2), 165-187. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90039-X

Ferrer, E., Shaywitz, B. A., Holahan, J. M., Marchione, K., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2010). Uncoupling of reading and IQ over time: empirical evidence for a definition of dyslexia. Psychological science, 21(1), 93–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609354084

Flitman, S., O’Grady, J., Cooper, V., & Grafman, J. (1997). PET imaging of maze processing. Neuropsychologia, 35(4), 409-420. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00086-3

Gallhofer, B., Bauer, U., Lis, S., Krieger, S., & Gruppe, H. (1996). Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: comparison of treatment with atypical antipsychotic agents and conventional neuroleptic drugs. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 6(S. 2), 13-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/0924-977X(96)00010-7

Gau, S. S. F., & Shang, C. Y. (2010). Executive functions as endophenotypes in ADHD: Evidence from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 51(7), 838-849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02215.x

Gilhooly, K.J. (2005). Working memory and planning. En R. Morris & G. Ward (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of planning (pp. 71-88). Psychology Press.

Goel, V., Grafman, J., Tajik, J., Gana, S., & Danto, D. (1997). A study of the performance of patients with frontal lobe lesions in a financial planning task. Brain, 120(10), 1805–1822. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.10.1805

Goldberg, T. E., Berman, K. F., Fleming, K., Ostrem, J., Van Horn, J. D., Esposito, G., Mattay, V. S., Gold, J. M., & Weinberger, D. R. (1998). Uncoupling cognitive workload and prefrontal cortical physiology: a PET rCBF study. NeuroImage, 7(4), 296–303. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0338

Goldstein, L. H., Bernard, S., Fenwick, P. B. C., Burgess, P. W., & McNeil, J. (1993). Unilateral frontal lobectomy can produce strategy application disorder. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 56, 274-276. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.56.3.274

Grafman, J., Spector, L., & Rattermann, M. J. (2005). Planning and the brain. En R. Morris & G. Ward (Ed.), The Cognitive Psychology of Planning (pp. 181-198). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203493564

Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. Cognitive Science, 3(4), 275–310. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0304_1

Holt, D. V., Wolf, J., Funke, J., Weisbrod, M., & Kaiser, S. (2013). Planning impairments in schizophrenia: Specificity, task independence and functional relevance. Schizophrenia Research, 149(1-3), 174-179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.06.018

Javadi, A. H., Emo, B., Howard, L. R., Zisch, F. E., Yu, Y., Knight, R., Pinelo Silva, J., & Spiers, H. J. (2017). Hippocampal and prefrontal processing of network topology to simulate the future. Nature communications, 8: 14652. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14652

Kaller, C. P., Reisert, M., Katzev, M., Umarova, R., Mader, I., Hennig, J., Weiller, C., & Köstering, L. (2015). Predicting planning performance from structural connectivity between left and right mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: moderating effects of age during postadolescence and midadulthood. Cerebral cortex, 25(4), 869–883. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht276

Karnath, H. O., Wallesch, C. W., & Zimmermann, P. (1991). Mental planning and anticipatory processes with acute and chronic frontal lobe lesions: A comparison of maze performance in routine and non-routine situations. Neuropsychologia, 29(4), 271-290. https://doi.org/ftpdd9

Kent, P. (2017). Fluid intelligence: A brief history. Applied neuropsychology. Child, 6(3), 193–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/21622965.2017.1317480

Kirsch, P., Lis, S., Esslinger, C., Gruppe, H., Danos, P., Broll, J., Wiltink, J., & Gallhofer, B. (2006). Brain activation during mental maze solving. Neuropsychobiology, 54(1), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1159/000095742

Koechlin, E. (2011). Frontal pole function: What is specifically human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(6), 241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.005

Koechlin, E. (2014). An evolutionary computational theory of prefrontal executive function in decision-making. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1655): 20130474. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0474

Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., & Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature, 399, 148-151. https://doi.org/10.1038/20178

Koechlin, E., & Hyafil, A. (2007). Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human decision-making. Science, 318(5850), 594-598. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1142995

Korn, C. W., & Bach, D. R. (2018). Heuristic and optimal policy computations in the human brain during sequential decision-making. Nature Communications, 9: 325. https://doi.org/gcw4px

Krieger, S., Lis, S., & Gallhofer, B. (2001). Cognitive subprocesses and schizophrenia. B. Maze tasks. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104(s408), 28–41. https://doi.org/dprpxn

Lazeron, R. H. C., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Machielsen, W. C. M., Scheltens, P., Witter, M. P., Uylings, H. B. M., & Barkhof, F. (2000). Visualizing brain activation during planning: The Tower of London test adapted for functional MR imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 21(8), 1407-1414. https://tinyurl.com/3y75dkhs

Lehto, J. E., Juujärvi, P., Kooistra, L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 59-80. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003321164627

Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.

Lima-Silva, T. B., Bahia, V. S., Carvalho, V. A., Guimarães, H., Caramelli, P., Damasceno, B., Bottino, C. M., Brucki, D., Nitrini, R. & Yassuda, M. S. (2013). Functional profile of patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) compared to patients with Alzheimer’s disease and normal controls. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 7(1), 96–103. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70100015

Lis, S., Krieger, S., Wilhelm, J., & Gallhofer, B. (2005). Feedback about previous action improves executive functioning in schizophrenia: An analysis of maze solving behaviour. Schizophrenia Research, 78(2-3), 243-250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.02.008

López-Villalobos, J. A., Serrano-Pintado, I., Andrés-De Llano, J. M., Sánchez-Mateos, J. D., Alberola-López, S., & Sánchez-Azón, M. I. (2010). Utilidad del test de Stroop en el trastorno por déficit de atencion/hiperactividad. Revista de neurología, 50(6), 333–340. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.5006.2009418

Lunt, L., Bramham, J., Morris, R. G., Bullock, P. R., Selway, R. P., Xenitidis, K., & David, A. S. (2012). Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and “Jumping to Conclusionsâ€: Bias or deficit? Journal of Neuropsychology, 6(1), 65–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1748-6653.2011.02005.x

Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: An introduction to neuropsychology. Basic books.

Mansouri, F. A., Egner, T., & Buckley, M. J. (2017). Monitoring Demands for Executive Control: Shared Functions between Human and Nonhuman Primates. Trends in Neurosciences, 40(1), 15-27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2016.11.001

Mansouri, F. A., Rosa, M. G. P., & Atapour, N. (2015). Working memory in the service of executive control functions. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9: 166. https://doi.org/gccxkj

Miles, S., Howlett, C. A., Berryman, C., Nedeljkovic, M., Moseley, G. L., & Phillipou, A. (2021). Considerations for using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test to assess cognitive flexibility. Behavior research methods, 53(5), 2083–2091. https://doi.org/jrwd

Milla, K., Bakhshipour, E., Bodt, B., & Getchell, N. (2019). Does movement matter? Prefrontal cortex activity during 2D vs. 3D performance of the tower of Hanoi puzzle. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 13:156. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00156

Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual review of neuroscience, 24, 167–202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & PRISMA Group (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS medicine, 6(7): e1000097. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

Nagahama, Y., Fukuyama, H., Yamauchi, H., Matsuzaki, S., Konishi, J., Shibasaki, H., & Kimura, J. (1996). Cerebral activation during performance of a card sorting test. Brain, 119(5), 1667–1675. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.5.1667

Neubert, F. X., Mars, R. B., Thomas, A. G., Sallet, J., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2014). Comparison of Human Ventral Frontal Cortex Areas for Cognitive Control and Language with Areas in Monkey Frontal Cortex. Neuron, 81(3), 700-713. https://doi.org/f5r9db

Newman, S. D., Carpenter, P. A., Varma, S., & Just, M. A. (2003). Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception. Neuropsychologia, 41(12), 1668–1682. https://doi.org/b3db53

Nitschke, K., Köstering, L., Finkel, L., Weiller, C., & Kaller, C. P. (2017). A Meta-analysis on the neural basis of planning: Activation likelihood estimation of functional brain imaging results in the Tower of London task. Human Brain Mapping, 38(1), 396–413. https://doi.org/jrwk

Oosterman, J. M., Wijers, M., & Kessels, R. P. C. (2013). Planning or something else? Examining neuropsychological predictors of zoo map performance. Applied Neuropsychology, 20(2), 103–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/09084282.2012.670150

Orr, J. M., & Weissman, D. H. (2009). Anterior cingulate cortex makes 2 contributions to minimizing distraction. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 703–711. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn119

Osherson, D., Perani, D., Cappa, S., Schnur, T., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1998). Distinct brain loci in deductive versus probabilistic reasoning. Neuropsychologia, 36(4), 369-376. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00099-7

Owen, A. M., Doyon, J., Petrides, M., & Evans, A. C. (1996). Planning and Spatial Working Memory: a Positron Emission Tomography Study in Humans. European Journal of Neuroscience, 8(2), 353–364. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01219.x

Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Semple, J., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1995). Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia, 33(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)00098-A

Pascualâ€Leone, A., Grafman, J., Clark, K., Stewart, M., Massaquoi, S., Lou, J. â€S, & Hallett, M. (1993). Procedural learning in Parkinson’s disease and cerebellar degeneration. Annals of Neurology, 34(4), 594-602. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.410340414

Peters, H. N., & Jones, F. D. (1951). Evaluation of group psychotherapy by means of performance tests. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 15(5), 363–367. https://doi.org/fdphbc

Porteus, S.D. (1959). The Maze Test and Clinical Psychology. Pacific.

Posner, MI & DiGirolamo GJ (1998). Executive attention: conflict, target detection, and cognitive control. En R Parasuraman (Ed.), The Attentive Brain (pp. 401–23). MIT.

Prabhakaran, V., Smith, J. A. L., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1997). Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: An fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 43-63. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1997.0659

Quilodran, R., Rothé, M., & Procyk, E. (2008). Behavioral shifts and action valuation in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuron, 57(2), 314–325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.031

Riggs, N. R., Spruijt-Metz, D., Sakuma, K. L., Chou, C. P., & Pentz, M. A. (2010). Executive cognitive function and food intake in children. Journal of nutrition education and behavior, 42(6), 398–403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2009.11.003

Rive, M. M., Koeter, M. W. J., Veltman, D. J., Schene, A. H., & Ruhé, H. G. (2016). Visuospatial planning in unmedicated major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder: Distinct and common neural correlates. Psychological Medicine, 46(11), 2313-2328. https://doi.org/f8zz6k

Schwartz, M. F., Reed, E. S., Montgomery, M., Palmer, C., & Mayer, N. H. (1991). The quantitative description of action disorganisation after brain damage: A case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8(5), 381-414. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643299108253379

Shallice, T. (1982). Specific Impairments of Planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 298(1089), 199–209. https://doi.org/cgxv89

Simon, H. A. (1975). The functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7(2), 268-288. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(75)90012-2

Sira, C. S., & Mateer, C. A. (2014). Executive Function. En M. J. Aminoff y R. B. Daroff (Eds.), Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences (pp. 239–242). https://doi.org/jrw5

Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.283.5408.1657

Spector, L., & Grafman, J. (1994). Planning, neuropsychology, and artificial intelligence: Cross fertilization. En F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 9, pp. 377–392). Elsevier Science.

Spellman, T., Svei, M., Kaminsky, J., Manzano-Nieves, G., & Liston, C. (2021). Prefrontal deep projection neurons enable cognitive flexibility via persistent feedback monitoring. Cell, 184(10), 2750–2766. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2021.03.047

Sternberg, R. J., & Ben-Zeev, T. (2001). Complex cognition: The psychology of human thought. Oxford University.

Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions. Cambridge University. https://doi.org/cbjdxm

Theeuwes J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: the effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & psychophysics, 49(1), 83–90. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211619

Tsujimoto, S., & Genovesio, A. (2017). Firing variability of frontal pole neurons during a cued strategy task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(1), 25-36. https://doi.org/jrw7

Tsujimoto, S., Genovesio, A., & Wise, S. P. (2010). Evaluating self-generated decisions in frontal pole cortex of monkeys. Nature Neuroscience, 13, 120-126. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2453

Tremblay, M., Lacroix, D., Chaput, Y., Fraïle, V., Lamer, R., & Albert, J.-M. (1994). Brain activation with a maze test: An EEG coherence analysis study in healthy subjects. NeuroReport, 5(18), 2449-2453. https://doi.org/bjzr9n

Unterrainer, J. M., Rahm, B., Kaller, C. P., Leonhart, R., Quiske, K., Hoppe-Seyler, K., Meier, C., Müller, C., & Halsband, U. (2004). Planning abilities and the Tower of London: is this task measuring a discrete cognitive function? Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 26(6), 846–856. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490509574

Urrútia, G., & Bonfill, X. (2013). La declaración PRISMA: un paso adelante en la mejora de las publicaciones de la Revista Española de Salud Pública. Revista española de salud pública, 87(2), 99–102. https://doi.org/10.4321/S1135-57272013000200001

van den Heuvel, O. A., Groenewegen, H. J., Barkhof, F., Lazeron, R. H., van Dyck, R., & Veltman, D. J. (2003). Frontostriatal system in planning complexity: a parametric functional magnetic resonance version of Tower of London task. NeuroImage, 18(2), 367–374. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00010-1

Wan, X., Cheng, K., & Tanaka, K. (2016). The neural system of postdecision evaluation in rostral frontal cortex during problem-solving tasks. ENeuro, 3(4), https://doi.org/ggmmcn

Ward, G. & Morris, R. G. (2005). Introduction to the psychology of planning. En R. Morris & G. Ward (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of planning (pp. 1-34). Psychology Press.

Will Crescioni, A., Ehrlinger, J., Alquist, J. L., Conlon, K. E., Baumeister, R. F., Schatschneider, C., & Dutton, G. R. (2011). High trait self-control predicts positive health behaviors and success in weight loss. Journal of health psychology, 16(5), 750–759. https://doi.org/crcstc

Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H. & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment of the dysexecutive syndrome. Thames Valley Test Company.

Wong, A. L., Haith, A. M., & Krakauer, J. W. (2015). Motor planning. Neuroscientist, 21(4), 385-398. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858414541484

SCImago Journal & Country Rank