Resumen
La evaluación del bienestar subjetivo y psicológico ha alcanzado considerable evidencia científica, principalmente a través de diferentes pruebas de autoinforme. Sin poner en cuestión este tipo de medición, lo cierto es que estas mediciones presentan la desventaja de estar basadas en declaraciones subjetivas y manipulables conscientemente por parte del sujeto. El objetivo de este trabajo es presentar un diseño bajo el paradigma del Test de Asociación Implícita de Greenwald y Farnham (2000) para aplicarlo a la medición implícita del bienestar subjetivo y psicológico. Así, se realizó un estudio transversal descriptivo con una muestra de 327 adultos (51,7% hombres; M = 38,69, DT = 11,98), que completaron un Test de Asociación Implícita para la medida objetiva del bienestar psicológico, y dos medidas autoinformadas para evaluar por un lado la felicidad y por otros posibles síntomas depresivos. Los resultados mostraron una buena fiabilidad del Test de Asociación Implícita, así como asociaciones débiles con las medidas de autoinforme. Una asociación implícita más fuerte con los afectos positivos que con los afectos negativos estuvo relacionada con una mayor felicidad subjetiva y menos síntomas depresivos. Estos resultados sugieren la posibilidad de completar la evaluación del bienestar usando medidas objetivas.
Citas
Alexander, R., Aragon, O. R., Bookwala, J., Cherbuin, N., Gatt, J. M., Kahrilas, I. J., … Styliadis, C. (2021). The neuroscience of positive emotions and affect: Implications for cultivating happiness and wellbeing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 121, 220–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.002
Bech, P., Rasmussen, N.-A., Olsen, L. R., Noerholm, V., y Abildgaard, W. (2001). The sensitivity and specificity of the Major Depression Inventory, using the Present State Examination as the index of diagnostic validity. Journal of Affective Disorders, 66(2), 159–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00309-8
Bond, T. N., y Lang, K. (2018). The Sad Truth About Happiness Scales. Journal of Political Economy. https://doi.org/10.1086/701679
Briñol, P., Horcajo, J., Becerra, A., Falces, C., y Sierra, B. (2002). Cambio de actitudes implícitas. Psicothema, 14(4), 771-775. https://tinyurl.com/ryhtyy87
Burgess, L., Nugent, S., Hunt, G., y Eslami, A. (2019). Pilot study: youth suicide assessment by Implicit Association Testing (IAT) in emergency psychiatry. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(10, S), S213–S214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.221
Byrne, B. M. (2013). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and programming. Routledge.
Caputo, A. (2017). Social desirability bias in self-reported well-being measures: Evidence from an online survey. Universitas Psychologica, 16(2), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy16-2.sdsw
Cárdenas, M., y Barrientos, J. (2008). Actitudes explícitas e implícitas hacia los hombres homosexuales en una muestra de estudiantes universitarios en Chile. Psykhe (Santiago), 17(2), 17-25. https://doi.org/10.4067/S0718-22282008000200002
Cárdenas, M., González, C., Calderón, C., y Lay, S. L. (2009). Medidas explícitas e implícitas de las actitudes hacia las mujeres. Revista Interamericana de Psicología/Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 43(3), 541-546. https://tinyurl.com/4xmax6wt
Corral-Frías, V., Lucas, M., Corral-Frias, N., Corral-Verdugo, V., y Tapia, C. (2019). Assessment of Affinity towards Diversity Using the Implicit Association Test and Self-Reports. Sustainability, 11, 5825. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11205825
Costantini, G., Perugini, M., Dentale, F., Barbaranelli, C., Alessandri, G., Vecchione, M., y Caprara, G. V. (2019). Assessing Positive Orientation with the Implicit Association Test. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35, 109-116. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000362
Da Victoria, M. S., Nascimento, A. L., y Fontenelle, L. (2011). Selection of visual stimuli for the Implicit Association Test for the Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder (IAT-OCD). Revista de Psiquiatria Clinica, 38(3), 102–105. https://doi.org/10.1590/S0101-60832011000300004
De las Cuevas y Catresana, C., y González de Rivera y Revuelta, J. (1992). Autoinformes y respuestas sesgadas. Anales de Psiquiatría, 8(9), 362 -366. https://tinyurl.com/5afjkn7w
Denegri, C. M., García, J. C., y González, R. N. (2015). Definición de bienestar subjetivo en adultos jóvenes profesionales chilenos. Un estudio con redes semánticas naturales. CES Psicología, 8(1), 77-97. https://doi.org/10.21615/3216
Deng, X., Ding, X., Cheng, C. y Chou, H. M. (2016). Feeling Happy and Sad at the Same Time? Subcultural Differences in Experiencing Mixed Emotions between Han Chinese and Mongolian Chinese. Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01692
Díaz, D., Horcajo, J., y Blanco, A. (2009). Development of an Implicit Overall Well-Being Measure Using the Implicit Association Test. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12(2), 604–617. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1138741600001979
Diener, E. (1994). Assessing subjective well-being: Progress and opportunities. Social Indicators Research, 31, 103–157. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01207052
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D., Oishi, S., y Biswas-Diener, R. (2009). New measures of well-being: Flourishing and positive and negative feelings. En E. Diener (Ed.), Assessing Well-Being (pp. 247-266), Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-2354-4_12
Diener, E., Horwitz, J., y Emmons, R. A. (1985). Happiness of the very wealthy. Social Indicators Research, 16, 263–274 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00415126
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., Pavot, W., y Gallagher, D. (1991). Response artifacts in the measurement of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Research, 24(1), 35-56. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00292649
Flores-Kanter, P.E., Muñoz-Navarro, R. y Medrano, L. A. (2018) Concepciones de la Felicidad y su relación con el Bienestar Subjetivo: un estudio mediante Redes Semánticas Naturales. Liberabit, 24(1), 115-130 https://doi.org/10.24265/liberabit.2018.v24n1.08
Fordyce, M.W. (1988). A Review of research on the happiness measures: A sixty second index of happiness and mental health. Social Indicators Research, 20, 355–381. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00302333
Frawley, A. (2015) Happiness Research: A Review of Critiques, Sociology Compass, 9, 62– 77, https://doi.org/10.1111/soc4.12236
Garín, M., Huenchuleo, J., Leal, N., Muñoz, A. M., y Rehbein, L. (2013). Actitudes implícitas hacia la violencia, conducta antisocial y consumo televisivo en estudiantes universitarios. Revista de Psicología, 22(2), 100-110. https://doi.org/10.5354/0719-0581.2013.30857
Gattol, V., Saaksjarvi, M., y Carbon, C.-C. (2011). Extending the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Assessing Consumer Attitudes Based on Multi-Dimensional Implicit Associations. Plos One, 6(1). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015849
Gómez-Benito, J., Hidalgo, M.D., y Guilera, G. (2010). El sesgo de los instrumentos de medición. Test justos. Papeles del Psicólogo, 3(1), 75-84. https://tinyurl.com/yjteftwy
Greenwald, A. G., y Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 1022–1038. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., y Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., y Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of personality and social psychology, 97(1), 17-41. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575
Hervás, G., y Vázquez, C. (2013). Construction and validation of a measure of integrative well-being in seven languages: The Pemberton Happiness Index. Health and quality of life outcomes, 11(1), 66–79, https://doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-11-66
Hills, P., y Argyle, M. (2002). The Oxford Happiness Questionnaire: A compact scale for the measurement of psychological well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 33(7), 1071-1082. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00213-6
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., y Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369-1385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205275613
Horcajo, J., Rubio, V. J., Aguado, D., Hernández, J. M. y Márquez, M. O. (2014). Using the Implicit Association Test to Assess Risk Propensity Self‐concept: Analysis of its Predictive Validity on a Risk‐taking Behaviour in a Natural Setting. European Journal of Personality, 28, 459–471. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1925
Hu, L. T., y Bentler, P.M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation modeling: a multidisciplinary journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Irving, L. H., y Smith, C. T. (2020). Measure what you are trying to predict: Applying the correspondence principle to the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 86, 103898. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.103898
Jost, J. T. (2019). The IAT Is Dead, Long Live the IAT: Context-Sensitive Measures of Implicit Attitudes Are Indispensable to Social and Political Psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 10–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721418797309
Kagan, J. (2012). Psychology's Ghosts: The Crisis in the Profession and the Way Back. Yale University Press.
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., y Stone, A. A. (2004). A Survey Method for Characterizing Daily Life Experience: The Day Reconstruction Method. Science, 306(5702), 1776-1780. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103572
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., Carroll, T. J., Karapetyan, A., Kaushik, N., Tomezsko, D., Greenwald, A. G., y Banaji, M. R. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569–586. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000364
Lyubomirsky, S., y Lepper, H. (1999). A Measure of Subjective Happiness: Preliminary Reliability and Construct Validation. Social Indicators Research, 46, 137-155. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006824100041
Muñiz, J. (1998). La medición de lo psicológico. Psicothema,10(1), 1-21. https://tinyurl.com/kykvx9m
Muñiz-Velázquez, J.A., Gomez-Baya, D. y Lopez-Casquete, M. (2017). Implicit and explicit assessment of materialism: Associations with happiness and depression. Personality and Individual Differences,116, 123–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.033.
Nebreda, C., y Aliaga, L. (2003). Major Depression Inventory-MDI. Hillerød: Psychiatric Research Unit. Mental Health Centre North Zealand.
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 166-180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271418
Olivos, P., y Aragonés, J. I. (2013). Test de asociaciones implícitas con la naturaleza: aplicación en España del “IAT-Nature”. Revista de Psicología Social, 28(2), 237-245. https://doi.org/10.1174/021347413806196672
Olsen, L. R., Jensen, D. V., Noerholm, V., Martiny, K., y Bech, P. (2003). The internal and external validity of theMajor Depression Inventory inmeasuring severity of depressive states. Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 351–356. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291702006724
Park, J., Uchida, Y. y Kitayama, S. (2016). Cultural variation in implicit independence: An extension of Kitayama et al. International Journal of Psychology, 51, 269–278. https://doi.org/10.1002/ijop.1215
Peterson, C. (2005). Authentic Happiness Inventory. https://tinyurl.com/ua6uv25m
Rath, D., Hallensleben, N., Glaesmer, H., Spangenberg, L., Strauss, M., Kersting, A., … Forkmann, T. (2018). Implicit Associations with Death: First Validation of the German Version of the Suicide Implicit Association Test (Suicide IAT). Psychotherapie Psychosomatik Medizinische Psychologie, 68(3–4), 109–117. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0043-105070
Riediger, M., Wrzus, C., y Wagner, G. G. (2014). Happiness is pleasant, or is it? Implicit representations of affect valence are associated with contrahedonic motivation and mixed affect in daily life. Emotion, 14, 950–961. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037711
Rohner, J., y Ewers, T. (2016). Trying to separate the wheat from the chaff: Construct- and faking-related variance on the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 243–258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0568-1
Sánchez, A.A. (2012). La utilización del Test de Asociación Implícita en los procesos electorales. Revista Justicia Electoral, 1(10), 267-292. https://tinyurl.com/usdyyu5j
Sanchez, A., Vazquez, C., Gomez, D., y Joormann, J. (2014). Gaze-fixation to happy faces predicts mood repair after a negative mood induction. Emotion, 14(1), 85-94. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034500
Salgado-Montejo, A., Salgado, C. J., Alvarado, J., y Spence, C. (2017). Simple lines and shapes are associated with, and communicate, distinct emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 31 (3), https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2015.1133401
Sriram, N., y Greenwald, A. G. (2009). The brief implicit association test. Experimental psychology, 56(4), 283-294. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.283
Tennant, R., Hiller, L., Fishwick, R., Platt, S., Joseph, S., Weich, S., … Stewart-Brown, S. (2007). The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-being Scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes, 5(1), 63. https://doi.org/10.1186/1477-7525-5-63
Van de Mortel, T. F. (2008). Faking it: Social desirability response bias in self-report research. Australian Journal of Advanced Nursing, 25(4), 40–48.
Walker, S.S., y Schimmack, U. (2008). Validity of a happiness Implicit Association Test as a measure of subjective well-being. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 2, 490-497. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2007.07.005
Warriner, A. B., Shore, D. I., Schmidt, L. A., Imbault, C. L., y Kuperman, V. (2017). Sliding into happiness: A new tool for measuring affective responses to words. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 71(1), 71-88. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000112
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., y Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
White, M. D. (2017). Judging the Efficacy and Ethics of Positive Psychology for Government Policymaking. En T. Lomas, N. Brown, y F. J. Eiroá-Orosa (Eds.), The Routledge International Handbook of Critical Positive Psychology (pp. 532-545). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315659794.ch35
Young, K., Kashdan, T., y Macatee, R. (2014). Strength balance and implicit strength measurement: New considerations for research on strengths of character. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10, 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2014.920406
Zevnik, L. (2014). The Discussion about the Universality of Happiness and the Promise of Neuroscience. Cultura-International Journal of Philosophy of Culture and Axiology, 11(1), 41–62. https://doi.org/10.5840/cultura20141113